Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 1437 – 1445
Emotions in consumer behavior: a hierarchical approach
Fleur J.M. Laros*, Jan-Benedict E.M. Steenkamp
Marketing Department, Tilburg University, P.O. Box 90153, 5000 LE Tilburg, The Netherlands
Received 1 December 2002; received in revised form 1 September 2003; accepted 1 September 2003
Abstract
A growing body of consumer research studies emotions evoked by marketing stimuli, products and brands. Yet, there has been a wide
divergence in the content and structure of emotions used in these studies. In this paper, we will show that the seemingly diverging research
streams can be integrated in a hierarchical consumer emotions model. The superordinate level consists of the frequently encountered general
dimensions positive and negative affect. The subordinate level consists of specific emotions, based on Richins’ (Richins, Marsha L.
Measuring Emotions in the Consumption Experience. J. Consum. Res. 24 (2) (1997) 127–146) Consumption Emotion Set (CES), and as an
intermediate level, we propose four negative and four positive basic emotions. We successfully conducted a preliminary test of this secondorder model, and compare the superordinate and basic level emotion means for different types of food. The results suggest that basic
emotions provide more information about the feelings of the consumer over and above positive and negative affect.
D 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Consumer emotions; Hierarchy of emotions; Positive and negative affect; Basic emotions; Specific emotions
1. Introduction
After a long period in which consumers were assumed to
make largely rational decisions based on utilitarian product
attributes and benefits, in the last two decades, marketing
scholars have started to study emotions evoked by marketing stimuli, products and brands (Holbrook and Hirschman,
1982). Many studies involving consumer emotions have
focused on consumers’ emotional responses to advertising
(e.g., Derbaix, 1995), and the mediating role of emotions on
the satisfaction of consumers (e.g., Phillips and Baumgartner, 2002). Emotions have been shown to play an important
role in other contexts, such as complaining (Stephens and
Gwinner, 1998), service failures (Zeelenberg and Pieters,
1999) and product attitudes (Dube et al., 2003). Emotions
are often conceptualized as general dimensions, like
positive and negative affect, but there has also been an
interest in more specific emotions. Within the latter stream
of research, some researchers use a comprehensive set of
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +31 13 4668212; fax: +31 13 4662875.
E-mail address: F.Laros@uvt.nl (F.J.M. Laros).
0148-2963/$ - see front matter D 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2003.09.013
specific emotions (Richins, 1997; Ruth et al., 2002). Other
researchers concentrate on one or several specific emotions,
such as surprise (e.g., Derbaix and Vanhamme, 2003), regret
(e.g., Inman and Zeelenberg, 2002; Tsiros and Mittal, 2000),
sympathy and empathy (Edson Escalas and Stern, 2003),
embarrassment (Verbeke and Bagozzi, 2003) and anger
(Bougie et al., 2003; Taylor, 1994).
Despite this emerging body of research, progress on the
use of emotions in consumer behavior has been hampered
by ambiguity about two interrelated issues, viz., the
structure and content of emotions (Bagozzi et al., 1999).
First, with regard to structure, some researchers examine all
emotions at the same level of generality (e.g., Izard, 1977),
whereas others specify a hierarchical structure in which
specific emotions are particular instances of more general
underlying basic emotions (Shaver et al., 1987; Storm and
Storm, 1987). Second, and relatedly, there is debate
concerning the content of emotions. Should emotions be
most fruitfully conceived as very broad general factors, such
as pleasure/arousal (Russell, 1980) or positive/negative
affect (Watson and Tellegen, 1985)? Alternatively, appraisal
theorists (see, e.g., Frijda et al., 1989; Roseman et al., 1996;
Smith and Lazarus, 1993) argue that specific emotions
1438
F.J.M. Laros, J.-B.E.M. Steenkamp / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 1437–1445
should not be combined in broad emotional factors, because
each emotion has a distinct set of appraisals. The confusion
concerning structure and content of emotions has hindered
the full interpretation and use of emotions in consumer
behavior theory and empirical research (Bagozzi et al.,
1999).
The purpose of our paper is twofold. First, we integrate
seemingly opposing research streams in psychology and
consumer behavior by developing a hierarchical model of
consumer emotions. We will show that the general
dimensions with positive and negative affect are the
superordinate and most abstract level at which emotions
can be defined. The subordinate level consists of specific
consumer emotions. We will develop an intermediate level
with basic emotions that links these two levels. Second, we
conduct a preliminary test of this proposed structure and
compare the means for positive and negative affect with
those of the basic emotions for four different food types.
2. Emotions in consumer research
This section will briefly discuss an illustrative set of
consumer studies on emotions (see Table 1 for an overview).
Several studies focused on the emotional responses to
ads. Holbrook and Batra (1987) developed their own
emotional scale based on an in-depth review of the
literature. They uncovered a pleasure, arousal and domination dimension in their data, and showed that these
emotions mediate consumer responses to advertising. Edell
and Burke (1987) also created their own emotion list and
found that feelings play an important role in the prediction
of the ad’s effectiveness. They proposed three factors: an
upbeat, negative, and warmth factor. Olney et al. (1991)
showed that the emotional dimensions pleasure and arousal
mediate the relation between ad content and attitudinal
components, and consequently viewing time of an ad.
They used part of Mehrabian and Russell’s (1974) scale.
Derbaix (1995) replicated the research of Edell and Burke
(1987) in a natural setting. His emotion words were based
on a prestudy, and uncovered a positive and negative
factor. Steenkamp et al. (1996) investigated the relations
between arousal potential, arousal, and ad evaluation, with
need for stimulation as a moderator. They based their
arousal dimension on the scale of Mehrabian and Russell
(1974).
In the satisfaction literature, Westbrook (1987) was one
of the first to investigate consumer emotional responses to
product/consumption experiences and their relationship to
several central aspects of postpurchase processes. Oliver
(1993) extended this work by showing that emotional
responses mediate the effects of product attributes on
satisfaction. Both studies relied on Izard’s (1977) taxonomy
of fundamental affects, and found positive and negative
affect as underlying emotion dimensions. Mano and Oliver
(1993) investigated the structural interrelationship among
evaluations, feelings, and satisfaction in the postconsumption experience. They combined Watson et al.’s (1988)
PANAS scale and Mano’s (1991) circumplex scale. Both
three dimensions—similar to the upbeat, negative, and
warmth factors of Edell and Burke (1987)—and two
dimensions—positive and negative affect—were uncovered,
but only the latter dimensions were used in the studies.
Dube and Morgan (1998) modeled trends in consumption
emotions and satisfaction in order to predict retrospective
global judgments of services. They used the PANAS scale
(Watson et al., 1988) and uncovered positive and negative
affect. Phillips and Baumgartner (2002) confirmed the
Table 1
Overview of consumer research using emotions as a main variable
Reference
Emotion measure used
Resulting structure
Edell and Burke (1987)
Holbrook and Batra (1987)
Westbrook (1987)
Olney et al. (1991)
Holbrook and Gardner (1993)
Mano and Oliver (1993)
Edell and Burke (1987)
Holbrook and Batra (1987)
Izard (1977)
Mehrabian and Russell (1974)
Russell et al. (1989)
Watson et al. (1988); Mano (1991)
Oliver (1993)
Derbaix (1995)
Steenkamp et al. (1996)
Nyer (1997)
Richins (1997)
Izard (1977)
Derbaix (1995)
Mehrabian and Russell (1974)
Shaver et al. (1987)
Richins (1997)
Dube and Morgan (1998)
Phillips and Baumgartner (2002)
Ruth et al. (2002)
Watson et al. (1988)
Edell and Burke (1987)
Shaver et al. (1987)
Smith and Bolton (2002)
Smith and Bolton (2002)
Upbeat, negative, and warm
Pleasure, arousal, and domination
Positive and negative affect
Pleasure and arousal
Pleasure and arousal
Upbeat, negative and warm
Positive and negative
Positive and negative affect
Positive and negative affect
Arousal
Anger, joy/satisfaction, and sadness
Anger, discontent, worry, sadness, fear, shame,
envy, loneliness, romantic love, love, peacefulness,
contentment, optimism, joy, excitement, and surprise
Positive and negative affect
Positive and negative affect
Love, happiness, pride, gratitude, fear, anger, sadness,
guilt, uneasiness, and embarrassment
Anger, discontent, disappointment, self-pity, and anxiety
F.J.M. Laros, J.-B.E.M. Steenkamp / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 1437–1445
importance of including positive and negative affect in
explaining satisfaction. Smith and Bolton (2002) investigated the role of consumer emotions in the context of
service failure and recovery encounters. They used content
analysis for the responses of the participants and grouped
the (negative) emotion words of consumers in five
categories.
Holbrook and Gardner (1993) investigated the relation
between the emotional dimensions pleasure and arousal and
the duration of a consumption experience, which was in
their case, listening to music. They used Russell et al.’s
(1989) Affect Grid to measure pleasure and arousal of the
musical stimuli.
Nyer (1997) and Ruth et al. (2002) focused on defining
the antecedents rather than the consequences of emotions.
1439
Nyer (1997) showed that the appraisals of goal relevance,
goal congruence, and coping potential are determinants of
several basic consumption emotions. These emotions were
mainly based on Shaver et al. (1987). Ruth et al. (2002)
explored the cognitive appraisals of situations and their
correspondence to 10 experienced emotions. They also used
emotions based on the hierarchical structure of Shaver et al.
(1987).
In summary, this overview shows that there is wide
divergence in the content of emotions studied in consumer
research. Studies often use different scales to measure
emotions and focus on different emotions. In spite of this,
consumer researchers frequently use, or exploratory data
analysis yields, a small number of dimensions (Bagozzi et al.,
1999). Among these, the classification of emotions in
Table 2
Emotion words
Negative emotion words
Positive emotion words
Aggravationa,b,c, Agitationa,b,c, Agonyb,c, Alarmb,c,d, Alienationb,
Anger a,b,c,d,e,f,g, Anguisha,b,c, Annoyancea,b,c,d,e,f,h, Anxietya,b,c,e,
Apologeticc, Apprehensiona,b,c, Aversione, Awfulc, Badc, Bashfulc,
Betrayalc, Bitternessa,b,c, Bluea,c,i, Botheredc, Cheerlessa, Confusedh,
Consternationc, Contemptb,c,e,g, Crankyc, Crossc, Crushedh, Cryc,
Defeatb, Deflateda,b, Defensivec, Dejectiona,b,c, Demoralizedc,
Depression a,b,c,d,h, Despairb,c, Devastationc, Differentc,
Disappointmenta,b,c,e,f, Discomfortc, Discontent a,c, Discouragedc,
Disenchantmentc, Disgusta,b,c,e,g,h, Dislikeb,c,g, Dismayb,c,
Displeasurea,b,c, Dissatisfieda,c, Distressa,b,c,d,g,i,j, Distrustc,e, Disturbedc,
Downa,c, Dreadb,c, Dumbc, Edgyc, Embarrassment a,b,c, Emptya,c,
Envy a,b,c, Exasperationb, Fear b,c,d,e,f,g,h,i,j, Fed-upa, Ferocityb, Flustereda,
Forlornc, Foolishc, Franticc, Frighta,b,c,h, Frustration a,b,c,d,f,g, Furya,b,c,
Gloomb,c,d,h, Glumnessb, Griefa,b,c,f, Grouchinessb,c,i, Grumpinessb,c,i,
Guilt b,c,e,g,j, Heart-brokena,c, Hateb,c,Hollowc, Homesickness a,b,c,
Hopelessnessb,c, Horriblec, Horrora,b,c,f, Hostility b,c,h,i,j, Humiliation b,c,
Hurta,b,c, Hysteriab, Impatienta,c, Indignantc, Inferiorc, Insecurityb,
Insultb,c, Intimidatedh, Iratea,c, Irkeda, Irritation a,b,c,h,j, Isolationb,c,
Jealousy a,b,c,e, Jitteryi,j, Joylessa, Jumpyc, Loathingb, Loneliness a,b,c,i,
Longingc, Lossc, Lovesicka, Lowa,c, Mada,c, Melancholyb,c, Misery a,b,c,d,
Misunderstoodc, Mopingc, Mortificationa,b, Mournfulc, Neglectb,c,
Nervousness a,b,c,i,j, Nostalgiac, Offendedh, Oppressedc, Outragea,b,c,
Overwhelmeda, Painc, Panic b,c, Petrifieda,c, Pitya,b,c, Puzzledh, Rageb,c,e,
Regreta,b,c,e,g, Rejectionb,c, Remorsea,b,c, Reproachfulc, Resentmenta,b,c,
Revulsionb, Ridiculousc, Rottenc, Sadness a,b,c,d,e,f,g,h,i, Scared a,c,h,j,
Scornb,c,i, Self-consciousc, Shame ,a,b,c,e,g,j, Sheepishc, Shocka,b,c, Shyc,
Sickeneda,c, Smallc, Sorrowa,b,c,e,i, Spiteb, Startlede,h, Strainedc, Stupidc,
Subduedc, Sufferingb,c, Suspensec, Sympathyb, Tenseness b,c,h, Terriblec,
Terrora,b,c, Threatenedh, Tormenta,b,c, Troubledc, Tremulousc, Uglyc,
Uneasinessa,b,c, Unfulfilled, Unhappinessa,b,c,i, Unpleasanth, Unsatisfiedc,
Unwantedc, Upseta,c,e,j, Vengefulnessb,c, Wantc, Wistfulc, Woeb,c,
Worry b,c, Wrathb,c, Yearningc
Acceptancec,h, Accomplishedc, Activei,j, Admirationc, Adorationb,c,
Affectionb,c, Agreementc, Alerth,j, Amazementb, Amusementa,b,c,
Anticipationb,c, Appreciationc, Ardentc, Arousala,b,d, Astonishmentb,d,i,
At easea,d, Attentiveh,j, Attractionb,c, Avidc, Blissb, Bravec,
Calm a,d, Caringb,c, Charmeda, Cheerfulnessa,b,c,h, Comfortablec,
Compassionb,c, Consideratec, Concernc, Contentment a,b,c,d,I,
Courageousc, Curioush, Delighta,b,c,d,h, Desireb,c, Determinedj,
Devotionc, Eagernessb,c, Ecstasya,b,c, Elationa,b,c,i, Empathyc, Enchantedc,
Encouraging c, Energeticf, Enjoymentb,c,f, Entertainedc, Enthrallmentb,
Enthusiasm b,c,e,f,i,j, Euphoriab,c, Excellentc, Excitement a,b,c,d,f,i,j,
Exhilarationb,f Expectantc, Exuberantc, Fantasticc, Fascinatede, Finec,
Fondnessb,c, Forgivingc, Friendlyc, Fulfillment c, Gaietyb,c, Generousc,
Gigglyc, Givingc, Gladnessa,b,c,d, Gleeb,c, Goodc, Gratitudec, Greatc,
Happiness a,b,c,d,e,f,h,i, Harmonyc, Helpfulc,h, Highc, Hope b,c,g, Hornyc,
Impressedc, Incrediblec, Infatuationb,c, Inspiredj, Interestedf,j, Jollinessb,
Jovialityb, Joy a,b,c,e,f,g, Jubilationb,c, Kindlyc,i, Lightheartedc, Likingb,c,g,
Longingb, Love a,b,c,e, Lustb,c, Merrimentc, Moveda, Nicec, Optimism b,
Overjoyeda,c, Passion a,b,c, Peaceful c,f, Peppyi, Perfectc, Pityc, Playfulc,
Pleasure a,c,d,f,i, Pridea,b,c,e,f,g,j, Protectivec, Raptureb, Reassuredc, Regardc,
Rejoicec, Relaxedc,d,f, Releasec, Relief a,b,c,e,f,g, Respectc, Reverencec,
Romantic c, Satisfactiona,b,c,d,f,i, Securec, Sensationalc, Sensitivec, Sensualc,
Sentimentality b,c, Serened,c, Sexy c, Sincerec, Strongi,j, Superc, Surpriseb,e,f,i,
Tendernessb,c, Terrificc, Thoughtfulc, Thrill a,b,c, Toucheda, Tranquilityc,
Triumphb, Trustc,h, Victoriousc, Warm-hearted c,i, Wonderfulc, Worshipc,
Zealb, Zestb
Note: The emotion words of Richins’ CES (1997) are in italics.
a
Morgan and Heise (1988).
b
Shaver et al. (1987).
c
Storm and Storm (1987).
d
Russell (1980).
e
Frijda et al. (1989).
f
Havlena et al. (1989).
g
Roseman et al. (1996).
h
Plutchik (1980).
i
Watson and Tellegen (1985).
j
Watson et al. (1988).
1440
F.J.M. Laros, J.-B.E.M. Steenkamp / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 1437–1445
positive and negative affect appears to be the most popular
conceptualization (see Table 1).
3. Positive and negative affect
Many papers acknowledge that positive and negative
affect are bever present in the experience of emotionsQ
(Diener, 1999, p. 804; see also Berkowitz, 2000; Watson
et al., 1999). We have content-analyzed 10 seminal studies
in psychology on emotions and emotion words (Frijda et al.,
1989; Havlena et al., 1989; Morgan and Heise, 1988;
Plutchik, 1980; Roseman et al., 1996; Russell, 1980; Shaver
et al., 1987; Storm and Storm, 1987; Watson and Tellegen,
1985; Watson et al., 1988). We were able to classify all
emotion words as either a positive or negative emotion (see
Table 2). Table 2 shows the emotion words and indicates
which studies included a particular word as a positive or
negative emotion word in their structure. The number of
references for each emotion word illustrates to what degree
researchers agree that this is an emotion word. For example,
the emotion words fear, sadness, and happiness appear
almost in every emotion structure, whereas others, like
mournful, forlorn, and zeal, are only mentioned occasionally.
In addition, Table 2 supports the notion that there are more
negative than positive emotion words (Morgan and Heise,
1988).
Yet, which of these many emotion words should be used to
measure consumer emotions? To address this issue, we can
use the important study by Richins (1997). Based on
extensive research, she constructed the Consumption Emotion Set (CES). This scale includes most, if not all, emotions
that can emerge in consumption situations and was developed
to distinguish the varieties of emotion associated with
different product classes. Table 2 reveals that the words
included in the CES (in italics) are among the most frequently
encountered words in the psychological emotion literature,
and can be easily divided in positive and negative affect.
Advantages of the division in positive and negative affect
are that (1) the model can be kept simple and (2) the
combination of a person’s positive and negative affect is
indicative of his/her attitude. The disadvantage is that
important distinctions among different positive and negative
emotions disappear (Lerner and Keltner, 2000; 2001). Thus,
more precise information about the feelings of the consumer
is lost (Bagozzi et al., 1999). Because different emotions can
have different behavioral consequences, it is important to
know, for example, whether a failure in a product or service
elicits feelings of anger or sadness. Both angry and sad
people feel that something wrong has been done to them,
but whereas sad people become inactive and withdrawn, the
angry person becomes more energized to fight against the
cause of anger (Shaver et al., 1987). Several studies have
shown how important it is to take into account differences
across emotions of the same valence (Lerner and Keltner,
2000; 2001; Zeelenberg and Pieters, 1999).
4. A hierarchy of consumer emotions
The research streams supporting the different emotion
structures (positive/negative vs. specific emotions) seem
opposing, but can in fact be seen as complementing. Shaver
et al. (1987) and Storm and Storm (1987) have suggested
that emotions can be grouped into clusters, yielding a
hierarchical structure. The most general, superordinate, level
consists of positive and negative affect. The next level is
considered as the basic emotion level, and the lowest,
subordinate, level consists of groups of individual emotions
that form a category named after the most typical emotion of
that category. Along the lines of the hierarchical structures
of Shaver et al. (1987) and Storm and Storm (1987), we thus
propose that consumer emotions can be considered at
different levels of abstractness.
Our hierarchy of consumer emotions distinguishes
between positive and negative affect at the superordinate
level. The specific consumer emotions based on Richins’
(1997) CES encompass the subordinate level. Which basic
emotions should constitute the intermediate level, however,
is less clear. Basic emotions are believed to be innate and
universal, but because there are different ways to conceive
emotions (facial, e.g., Ekman, 1992; biosocial, e.g., Izard,
1992; brain, e.g., Panksepp, 1992), there is also disagreement about which emotions are basic (Turner and Ortony,
1992). Ortony and Turner (1990) have shown that 14
different emotion theorists proposed 14 different sets of
basic emotions. Table 3 shows the usage frequency of the
basic emotions in the different structures reviewed by
Ortony and Turner (1990). With few exceptions, the basic
Table 3
Basic emotions in the psychological literature (adapted from Ortony and
Turner, 1990)
Acceptancea, Angera,b,c,d,e,f,g,h,i, Anticipationa, Anxietyf,h,j, Aversionb,
Contemptd,i, Contentmenth, Courageb, Dejectionb, Desireb,k, Despairb,
Disgusta,c,d,e,f,h,i, Distressd,i, Elatione, Expectancyl, Feara,b,c,d,e,g,h,i,l,m,n,
Grief m, Guiltd, Happinessf,h,k,o, Hateb, Hopeb, Hostilityh, Interestd,k,
Joya,c,d,g,i,j, Likingh, Loveb,g,h,m,n, Painh,p, Panicl, Pleasurep, Prideh,
Ragej,l,m,n, Sadnessa,b,c,f,g,h,o, Shamed,h,i, Sorrowk, Subjectione,
Surprisea,c,d,i,k, Tendere, Wondere,k
a
Plutchik (1980).
Arnold (1960).
c
Ekman et al. (1982).
d
Izard (1971).
e
McDougal (1926).
f
Oatley and Johnson-Laird (1987)
g
Shaver et al. (1987).
h
Storm and Storm (1987).
i
Tomkins (1984).
j
Gray (1982).
k
Frijda (1986).
l
Panksepp (1982).
m
James (1884).
n
Watson (1930).
o
Weiner and Graham (1984).
p
Mowrer (1960).
b
F.J.M. Laros, J.-B.E.M. Steenkamp / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 1437–1445
emotions from Table 3 are among the most frequently
mentioned emotion words in Table 2.
To develop a set of basic consumer emotions, we draw
on the hierarchical structures of Shaver et al. (1987) and
Storm and Storm (1987), and Table 3. Some basic emotion
words are mentioned in most of the structures (see Table 3).
These are anger, fear, love, sadness, disgust, joy, and
surprise. Anger, fear, love and sadness are basic emotions in
both the structures of Shaver et al. (1987) and Storm and
Storm (1987), and will be retained in our structure. Disgust
is not included in the structure of Richins (1997) and
therefore excluded as a basic consumption emotion.
Surprise was excluded for several reasons. First, it is a
neutral emotion (Storm and Storm, 1987) and therefore
impossible to classify as a positive or negative emotion.
Second, when participants were required to list emotions,
surprise was hardly mentioned (Fehr and Russell, 1984).
Following Storm and Storm (1987), we added the
emotion shame to the basic negative emotions. Anger,
sadness, and fear are all emotions elicited by situations
caused by others or circumstances, whereas shame is caused
by a negative action of consumers themselves (Roseman et
al., 1996).
The positive emotions can be roughly divided in interpersonal emotions and emotions without interpersonal
reference (Storm and Storm, 1987). The interpersonal
emotions are covered by love and its specific emotion
words, but there are distinct differences between the
emotions that are not interpersonal. Following Storm and
Storm (1987), we therefore replaced the more general term
joy by the basic emotions contentment, happiness, and
pride. Contentment is low in arousal and passive, whereas
happiness is higher in activity and a reactive positive
emotion. Pride, on the other hand, concerns feelings of
superiority. Due to these differences, we argue that it is
better to include these basic emotions separately rather than
all under one large basic emotion of joy.
Our proposed hierarchy thus consists of three levels: the
superordinate level with positive and negative affect, the
basic level with four positive and four negative emotions,
and the subordinate level with specific emotions. The final
1441
result can be seen in Fig. 1. Next, we will conduct a
preliminary test of our hypothesized structure.
5. Method
5.1. Sample and procedure
Data were collected in a nationally representative sample
among 645 Dutch consumers using a questionnaire. The
market research agency GfK carried out the data collection.
Of the respondents, 53.6% were women, 58.3% were
responsible for the daily grocery shopping, and 69.1% were
the main wage earner of the household. The average
household size was 2.39 persons and all levels of education
and income were represented. The average age was 48
years and ranged between 16 and 91 with a fairly normal
spread.
Respondents were asked to indicate to what extent they
experience 33 specific emotions for one (randomly
assigned) type of food (genetically modified food, functional food, organic food, or regular food). Thus, we
measure emotions at a general, product-type level of
categorization. In The Netherlands, these types of foods
are widely known, the exception being functional foods
(this was confirmed in discussions with industry experts).
Therefore, respondents who rated their emotions for functional foods received additional explanation: bFunctional
foods are food products that have been enriched or
modified. The reason for this is to make the product
healthier or to prevent diseases (e.g., milk with extra
calcium, margarine with additives to lower the cholesterol
level)Q.
5.2. Measures
With some exceptions, the emotion words shown in Fig.
1 were used. Emotions were rated on a five-point Likert
scale ranging from I feel this emotion not at all (1) to I feel
this emotion very strongly (5). In our empirical test, we
omitted the basic emotions bloveQ and bprideQ, and the
Fig. 1. Hierarchy of consumer emotions.
1442
F.J.M. Laros, J.-B.E.M. Steenkamp / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 1437–1445
emotion words benviousQ and bjealousQ. bLoveQ is demonstrated to be mainly experienced in the case of sentimental
products, like mementos and gifts (Richins, 1997). The
latter three emotions are interpersonal and less applicable in
the case of widely available food. The emotion bprideQ
generally occurs when a consumer feels superior compared
to another person, whereas the emotions benvyQ and
bjealousyQ occur when consumers feel that another person
has something more or better than them. Thus, the basic
emotions in our analyses are as follows: anger, fear, sadness,
shame, contentment, and happiness, measured in total by 33
specific emotion words.
5.2. Stability of the emotions structure across food types
Before we can test our second-order hierarchical model
of consumer emotions, we have to establish whether we can
pool the data across the four food types. We do this in two
ways. First, we assess whether principal component analysis
yields the same factor structure in each of the four food
groups. The Bartlett’s test of sphericity is significant for all
four foods, and the measure of sampling adequacy ranges
between .86 (organic food) and .92 (genetically modified
food), which means that principal component analysis can
be applied. The scree test indicated two factors in all four
groups, explaining between 48% (regular food) and 60%
(genetically modified food) of the variance. The factor
structures (after rotation) were highly similar, Tucker’s
congruence coefficient always being greater than .95
( P b.01; Cattell, 1978).
A second way to assess the similarity of the four food
groups is to test for the invariance of the covariance matrices
across the four groups using LISREL 8.50 (Steenkamp and
Baumgartner, 1998). The fit was good, given the large
sample and high number of degrees of freedom (Baumgartner and Homburg, 1996): v 2(1683)=3845.90 ( Pb.001);
CFI=.86; TLI=.82. Hence, we can pool the data across the
different food types.
6. Results
6.1. Testing the proposed model
We used LISREL 8.50 to test the proposed hierarchical
emotions model. The standardized parameter estimates of
the second-order factor analysis are reported in Fig. 2.
Model fit is acceptable: v 2(490)=3036.79 ( Pb.001),
CFI=.84, TLI=.83. Although the v 2 was highly significant
(not unexpected given the large sample size; Anderson and
Gerbing, 1988), other indicators suggest reasonable model
fit, especially considering that fit is adversely affected by
model complexity (Baumgartner and Homburg, 1996;
Bollen, 1989; Bone et al., 1989). In addition, the fit
measures are in line with simulation results (see Gerbing
and Anderson, 1993 for a review) and compare favorably to
other models with similar degrees of freedom (e.g.,
Netemeyer et al., 1991; Richins and Dawson, 1992; Wong
et al., 2003).
All factor loadings were significant at P b.001, the
average loading being .73. Only the factor loading of the
emotion nostalgia on the basic emotion sadness was below
.40. A possible explanation for this is that nostalgia involves
complex emotional responses and can have both a positive
and a negative connotation (Holak and Havlena, 1998). The
correlation between the second-order factors positive and
negative affect was significant (r= .35, Pb.01), confirming
earlier results found in consumer research (e.g., Westbrook,
1987; Phillips and Baumgartner, 2002).
These results support the convergent and discriminant
validity of our model (Steenkamp and Van Trijp, 1991). The
reliability of our measures was high. Cronbach alphas were
a=.94 and a=.95 for the dimensions positive and negative
affect, respectively. The basic emotions yielded the following reliabilities: anger (a=.88), fear (a=.88), sadness
(a=.76), shame (a=.74), contentment (a=.86), and happiness (a=.92).
6.2. Comparison of the superordinate level with the basic
emotions
Although the emotion structure is similar for the four
food groups, that does not imply that the various foods
evoke the same emotional intensity. Table 4 provides the
mean scores for the superordinate dimensions positive and
negative affect and for the basic emotions.
ANOVA with multiple comparisons (LSD) was used to
investigate whether the mean values across food groups are
significantly different. Participants experience significantly
more negative affect and less positive affect for genetically
modified foods than for the other food groups. Yet, the
basic emotions show differences among the food types that
would have been lost if only positive and negative affect
had been considered. Both the basic emotions fear and
contentment contain additional subtle distinctions across the
food groups. The negative affect experienced by consumers
is similar for functional, organic, and regular food. Yet,
consumers feel a lot more fearful concerning functional
food than for organic and regular food. Concerning the
positive emotions, contentment has very low values for
organic food compared to functional and regular food.
These nuances, however, are wiped away for positive
affect.
To demonstrate the usefulness of basic emotions for
understanding the consumer’s feelings, we will take a closer
look at one of the food groups. Genetically modified food
represents a controversial topic in contemporary society,
and previous research (e.g., Bredahl, 2001) has shown that
consumers have a rather negative attitude towards this type
of food. The scores on negative and positive affect support
this, but the basic emotions indicate more clearly how
consumers feel. Participants do not feel sad or ashamed, but
F.J.M. Laros, J.-B.E.M. Steenkamp / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 1437–1445
1443
Fig. 2. Results of second-order factor analysis.
are very angry and afraid. This means that they feel
energized and powerful rather than inactive, and feel that
they themselves are not to be blamed, but someone else is.
In addition, genetically modified food elicits strong
associations of risk and uncertainty leading to feelings of
fear.
Table 4
Differences in the intensity of the superordinate and basic emotions for the food groups
Emotion
GMF
Functional
Organic
Regular
F
P value
Negative affect
Anger
Sadness
Fear
Shame
Positive affect
Contentment
Happiness
1.99a
2.19a
1.79a
2.16a
1.65a
1.68a
1.82a
1.64a
1.45b
1.51b
1.46b
1.57b
1.32b
2.41b,c
2.69b
2.32b
1.43b
1.47b
1.47b
1.40c
1.29b
2.32c
2.40c
2.29b
1.46b
1.55b
1.47b
1.43c
1.31b
2.48b
2.81b
2.37b
31.25
34.49
11.99
46.06
11.30
40.09
47.38
33.64
b.001
b.001
b.001
b.001
b.001
b.001
b.001
b.001
Note: Different supercripts reflect a significant difference of the intensity at a p-value b0.05.
1444
F.J.M. Laros, J.-B.E.M. Steenkamp / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 1437–1445
7. Conclusion
References
Based on our literature review, we concluded that despite
the different ways to measure emotions, positive and negative
affect are frequently employed as general emotion dimensions. Important nuances, however, are lost if emotions of the
same valence are collapsed together. This paper therefore
proposed a hierarchical model of consumer emotions (Fig. 1)
to integrate the different research streams concerning emotion
content and structure. This model specifies emotions at three
levels of generality. At the superordinate level, it distinguishes between positive and negative affect. This is
generally considered to be the most abstract level at which
emotions can be experienced (e.g., Berkowitz, 2000; Diener,
1999). At the level of basic emotions, we specify four positive
(contentment, happiness, love, and pride) and four negative
(sadness, fear, anger, and shame). At the subordinate level,
we distinguish between 42 specific emotions based on
Richins’ (1997) CES. Our empirical study provides support
for the proposed model and suggests that the basic emotions
allow for a better understanding of the consumers’ feelings
concerning certain food products compared to only positive
and negative affect. Note that not in all situations this model
need be used as a whole. Dependent on the research question,
only part of the model may be used. However, even in such
cases, the researcher can still relate his/her specific results to
the broader structure of our emotions. This makes it easier for
emotions research to cumulatively build on each other and to
identify gaps in our knowledge.
Our study has several limitations, which offer avenues
for future research. First, we excluded two basic emotions
(love and pride) from our empirical analysis. Future
research is needed to validate the whole hierarchy of
emotions, and to test our model on other products and
services. Second, future research can expand the set of
specific consumer emotions. Possible candidates include the
negative emotions regret and disappointment that recently
received a great deal of attention in consumer research (e.g.,
Inman and Zeelenberg, 2002; Tsiros and Mittal, 2000;
Zeelenberg and Pieters, 1999). Regret stems from bad
decisions, whereas disappointment originates from disconfirmed expectancies (Zeelenberg and Pieters, 1999). We
thus propose that regret can be positioned under the basic
emotion shame, and disappointment under the basic
emotion sadness (Zeelenberg et al., 1998), but future
research should investigate this.
Third, future research can investigate whether the set of
basic emotions has greater explanatory power than positive
and negative affect. Our exploratory analysis indicates this,
but future research should test this hypothesis. Fourth, we
tested our emotions model in The Netherlands. The further
advancement of consumer research as an academic
discipline requires that the validity of our theories and
measures and their degree of general validity and boundary
conditions be tested in different countries (Steenkamp and
Burgess, 2002).
Anderson James C, Gerbing David W. Structural equation modeling in
practice: a review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol Bull
1988;103(3):411 – 23.
Bagozzi Richard P, Gopinath Mahesh, Nyer Prashanth U. The role of
emotions in marketing. J Acad Mark Sci 1999;27(2):184 – 206.
Baumgartner Hans, Homburg Christian. Applications of structural equation
modeling in marketing and consumer research: a review. Int J Res Mark
1996;13(2):139 – 61.
Berkowitz Leonard. Causes and consequences of feelings. New York7
Cambridge University Press; 2000.
Bollen Kenneth A. Structural equations with latent variables. New York7
Wiley; 1989.
Bone Paula Fitzgerald, Sharma Subhash, Shimp Terence. A bootstrap
procedure for evaluating goodness-of-fit indices of structural equation
and confirmatory factor models. J Mark Res 2003;26(1):105 – 11.
Bougie Roger, Pieters Rik, Zeelenberg Marcel. Angry customers don’t
come back, they get back: the experience and behavioral implications of
anger and dissatisfaction in services. Journal of the Academy of
Marketing Science; 2003;31(4):377 – 393.
Bredahl Lone. Determinants of consumer attitudes and purchase intentions
with regard to genetically modified foods—results of a cross-national
survey. J Consum Policy 2001;24:23 – 61.
Cattell RB. The scientific use of factor analysis in behavioral and life
sciences. New York7 Plenum; 1978.
Derbaix Christian M. The impact of affective reactions on attitudes toward
the advertisement and the brand: a step toward ecological validity. J
Mark Res 1995;32(4):470 – 9.
Derbaix Christian M, Vanhamme Joelle. Inducing word-of-mouth by
eliciting surprise—a pilot investigation. J Econ Psychol 2003;24:
99 – 116.
Diener Ed. Introduction to the special section on the structure of emotion. J
Pers Soc Psychol 1999;76(5):803 – 4.
Dube Laurette, Morgan Michael S. Capturing the dynamics of in-process
consumption emotions and satisfaction in extended service transactions.
Int J Res Mark 1998;15:309 – 20.
Dube Laurette, Cervellon Marie-Cecile, Jingyuan Han. Should consumer
attitudes be reduced to their affective and cognitive bases? Validation of
a hierarchical model. Int J Res Mark 2003;20:259 – 72.
Edell Julie A, Burke Marian C. The power of feelings in understanding
advertising effects. J Consum Res 1987;14(4):421 – 33.
Edson Escalas Jennifer, Stern Barbara B. Sympathy and empathy: emotional
responses to advertising dramas. J Consum Res 2003;29(4):566 – 78.
Ekman Paul. Are there basic emotions? Psychol Rev 1992;99(3):550 – 3.
Fehr Beverly, Russell James A. Concept of emotion viewed from a
prototype perspective. J Exp Psychol Gen 1984;113:464 – 86.
Frijda Nico H, Kuipers Peter, Ter Schure Elisabeth. Relations among
emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. J Pers Soc Psychol
1989;57(2):212 – 28.
Gerbing David W, Anderson James C. Monte Carlo evaluations of
goodness-of-fit indices for structural equation models. In: Bollen
Kenneth A, Long J. Scott, editors. Testing structural equation models.
Newbury Park, CA7 Sage; 1993. p. 40 – 65.
Havlena William J, Holbrook Morris B, Lehmann Donald R. Assessing the
validity of emotional typologies. Psychol Market 1989;6:97 – 112.
Holak Susan L, Havlena William J. Feelings, fantasies, and memories: an
examination of the emotional components of nostalgia. J Bus Res
1998;42(3):217 – 27.
Holbrook Morris B, Batra Rajeev. Assessing the role of emotions as
mediators of consumer responses to advertising. J Consum Res 1987;
14(3):404 – 20.
Holbrook Morris B, Gardner Meryl P. An approach to investigating the
emotional determinants of consumption durations: why do people
consume what they consume for as long as they consume it? J Consum
Psychol 1993;2(2):123 – 42.
F.J.M. Laros, J.-B.E.M. Steenkamp / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 1437–1445
Holbrook Morris B, Hirschman Elizabeth C. The experiential aspects of
consumption: consumer fantasies, feelings, and fun. J Consum Res
1982;9(2):132 – 40.
Inman Jeffrey J, Zeelenberg Marcel. Regret in repeat purchase versus
switching decisions: the attenuating role of decision justifiability. J
Consum Res 2002;29(1):116 – 28.
Izard Carroll. Human emotions. New York7 Plenum; 1977.
Izard Carroll. Basic emotions, relations among emotions, and emotion–
cognition relations. Psychol Rev 1992;99(3):561 – 5.
Lerner Jennifer S, Keltner Dacher. Beyond valence: toward a model of
emotion-specific influences on judgment and choice. Cogn Emot
2000;14(4):473 – 93.
Lerner Jennifer S, Keltner Dacher. Fear, anger and risk. J Pers Soc Psychol
2001;81(1):146 – 59.
Mano Haim. The structure and intensity of emotional experiences: method
and context convergence. Multivariate Behav Res 1991;26(3):389 – 411.
Mano Haim, Oliver Richard L. Assessing the dimensionality and structure
of the consumption experience: evaluation, feeling, and satisfaction. J
Consum Res 1993;20(3):451 – 66.
Mehrabian Albert, Russell James A. An approach to environmental
psychology. Cambridge, MA7 MIT Press; 1974.
Morgan Rich L., Heise David. Structure of emotions. Soc Psychol Q
1988;51(1):19 – 31.
Netemeyer Richard G, Durvasula Srinivas, Lichtenstein Donald. A crossnational assessment of the reliability and validity of the CETSCALE. J
Mark Res 1991;28(3):320 – 7.
Nyer Prashanth U. A study of the relationships between cognitive
appraisals and consumption emotions. J Acad Mark Sci 1997;25(4):
296 – 304.
Oliver Richard L. Cognitive, affective, and attribute bases of the satisfaction
response. J Consum Res 1993;20(3):418 – 30.
Olney Thomas J, Holbrook Morris B., Batra Rajeev. Consumer responses to
advertising: the effects of ad content, emotions, and attitude toward the
ad on viewing time. J Consum Res 1991;17(4):440 – 53.
Ortony Andrew, Turner Terence J. What’s basic about basic emotions?
Psychol Rev 1990;97(3):315 – 31.
Panksepp Jaak. A crititical role for baffective neuroscienceQ in resolving
what is basic about basic emotions. Psychol Rev 1992;99(3):554 – 60.
Phillips Diane M, Baumgartner Hans. The role of consumption emotions in
the satisfaction response. J Consum Psychol 2002;12(3):243 – 52.
Plutchik Robert. Emotion: a psychoevolutionary synthesis. New York7
Harper and Row; 1980.
Richins Marsha L. Measuring emotions in the consumption experience. J
Consum Res 1997;24(2):127 – 46.
Richins Marsha L, Dawson Scott. A consumer values orientation for
materialism and its measurement: measure development and validation.
J Consum Res 1992;19(3):303 – 16.
Roseman Ira J, Antoniou Ann A, Jose Paul J. Appraisal determinants of
emotions: constructing a more accurate and comprehensive theory.
Cogn Emot 1996;10(3):241 – 77.
Russell James A., Weiss A., Mendelsohn G.A.. Affect grid - A single-item
scale of pleasure and arousal. J Pers Soc Psychol 1989;57(3):493 – 502.
Russell James A. A circumplex model of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol 1980;
39(6):1161 – 78.
Ruth Julie A, Brunel Frederic F, Otnes Cele C. Linking thoughts to feelings:
investigating cognitive appraisals and consumption emotions in a
mixed-emotions context. J Acad Mark Sci 2002;30(1):44 – 58.
1445
Shaver Philip, Schwartz Judith, Kirson Donald, O’Conner Cary. Emotion
knowledge: further exploration of a prototype approach. J Pers Soc
Psychol 1987;52:1061 – 86.
Smith Amy K, Bolton Ruth N. The effects of customers’ emotional
responses to service failures on their recovery effort evaluations and
satisfaction judgments. J Acad Mark Sci 2002;30(1):5 – 23.
Smith Craig A, Lazarus Richard S. Appraisal components, core relational
themes, and emotions. Cogn Emot 1993;7:295 – 323.
Steenkamp Jan-Benedict EM, Baumgartner Hans. Assessing measurement
invariance in cross-national consumer research. J Consum Res 1998;
25(1):78 – 90.
Steenkamp Jan-Benedict E.M., Burgess Steven M.. Optimum stimulation
level and exploratory consumer behavior in an emerging consumer
market. Int J Res Mark 2002;19:131 – 50.
Steenkamp Jan-Benedict EM, Van Trijp Hans CM. The use of LISREL in
validating marketing constructs. Int J Res Mark 1991;8(4):283 – 99.
Steenkamp Jan-Benedict EM, Baumgartner Hans, Van der Wulp Elise. The
relationships among arousal potential, arousal and stimulus evaluation,
and the moderating role of need for stimulation. Int J Res Mark
1996;13:319 – 29.
Stephens Nancy, Gwinner Kevin P. Why don’t some people complain? A
cognitive–emotive process model of consumer complaint behavior. J
Acad Mark Sci 1998;26(4):172 – 89.
Storm Christine, Storm Tom. A taxonomic study of the vocabulary of
emotions. J Pers Soc Psychol 1987;53:805 – 16.
Taylor Shirley. Waiting for service: the relationship between delays and
evaluations of service. J Mark 1994;58(2):56 – 69.
Tsiros Michael, Mittal Vikas. Regret: a model of its antecedents and
consequences in consumer decision making. J Consum Res
2000;26(4):401 – 17.
Turner Terence J, Ortony Andrew. Basic emotions: can conflicting criteria
converge? Psychol Rev 1992;99(3):566 – 71.
Verbeke Willem, Bagozzi Richard P. Exploring the role of self- and
customer-provoked embarrassment in personal selling. Int J Res Mark
2003;20:233 – 58.
Watson David, Tellegen Auke. Toward a consensual structure of mood.
Psychol Bull 1985;98:219 – 35.
Watson David, Clark Lee Anna, Tellegen Auke. Development and
validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS
scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988;54:1063 – 70.
Watson David, Wiese David, Vaidya Jatin, Tellegen Auke. The two general
activation systems of affect: structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. J Pers Soc Psychol 1999;
76(5):820 – 38.
Westbrook Robert A. Product/consumption-based affective responses and
post-purchase processes. J Mark Res 1987;24:258 – 70.
Wong Nancy, Rindfleisch Aric, Burroughs James E. Do reverse-worded
items confound measures in cross-cultural consumer research: the case
of the material values scale. J Consum Res 2003;30(1):72 – 91.
Zeelenberg Marcel, Pieters Rik. Comparing service delivery to what might
have been. J Serv Res 1999;2(1):86 – 97.
Zeelenberg Marcel, Van Dijk Wilco W, Manstead Antony SR, Van der Pligt
Joop. The experience of regret and disappointment. Cogn Emot
1998;12(2):221 – 30.